How Much Guidance Do Students Need? An Intervention Study on Kindergarten Mathematics with Manipulatives

Authors

https://doi.org/10.17583/ijep.2018.3672

Keywords:


Downloads

Abstract

Research has shown that the efficacy of learning with manipulatives (e.g., fingers, blocks, or coins) is affected by multiple variables, including the amount of guidance teachers provide during learning. However, there is no consensus on how much guidance is necessary when learning with manipulatives. The goal of this study was to examine the optimal level of guidance during instruction with manipulatives. The focus was on the timing and level of guidance. The researcher taught students a lesson on counting from one to 10 with pennies and nickel strips. Kindergarten students were taught over five consecutive days in one of four conditions: high guidance, low guidance, high guidance that transitioned to low guidance, and low guidance that transitioned to high guidance. Results showed no difference in learning across the conditions. These results provide valuable information to teachers on the areas of mathematics that do not require the effort of high guidance. 

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Author Biographies

Erin Maria Horan, Association of American Colleges and Universities

Dr. Erin Horan is a Postdoctoral Research Analyst in the Office of Quality, Curriculum, and Assessment at the Association of American Colleges and Universities (AAC&U). She holds a PhD from the University of Georgia in Educational Psychology, Applied Cognition and Development. This study is from her dissertation research performed under the direction of her advisor, Dr. Martha Carr. Her current research at AAC&U focuses on assessment and accreditation in higher education, specifically related to the validity of assessment techniques that utilize authentic student work.

Martha M. Carr, University of Georgia

Dr. Martha Carr was a University of Georgia (UGA) Aderhold Distinguished Professor of Educational Psychology and Research Fellow of the UGA Institute of Behavioral Research. Marty studied the factors that promote or inhibit mathematics achievement at various ages and educational stages. Her most recent work studied the impact of spatial skills on the developmental trajectory of mathematics competency. Her work focused on closing the mathematics achievement gap for students and her work has impacted, and will continue to impact, future research, teachers, and students.

References

Baroody, A. J., Purpura, D. J., Eiland, M. D., & Reid, E. E. (2015). The impact of highly and minimally guided discovery instruction on promoting the learning of reasoning strategies for basic add-1 and doubles combinations. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 30(Part A), 93–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.09.003

Google Scholar Crossref

Bruner, J. S. (1961). The act of discovery. Harvard Educational Review, 31(1), 21–32.

Google Scholar Crossref

Bruner, J. S. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, Mass., Belknap Press of Harvard University.

Google Scholar Crossref

Carbonneau, K.J., Marley, S.C., & Selig, J.P. (2013). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of teaching mathematics with concrete manipulatives. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 380-400. doi: 10.1037/a0031084

Google Scholar Crossref

Carbonneau, K., & Marley, S. (2015). Instructional guidance and realism of manipulatives influence preschool children’s mathematics learning. The Journal of Experimental Education, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2014.989306

Google Scholar Crossref

Chen, O., Kalyuga, S., & Sweller, J. (2015). The worked example effect, the generation effect, and element interactivity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 689–704.

Google Scholar Crossref

Clarke, B., & Shinn, M. (2002). Test of early numeracy (ten): Administration and scoring of aimsweb early numeracy measures for use with aimsweb. Eden Prairie, MN: Edformation Inc.

Google Scholar Crossref

Cobb, P. (1995). Cultural tools and mathematical learning: A case study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(4), 362-385.

Google Scholar Crossref

Darch, C., Carnine, D., & Gersten, R. (1984). Explicit instruction in mathematics problem solving. Journal of Educational Research, 77(6), 351.

Google Scholar Crossref

DeLoache, J. S. (2000). Dual representation and young children’s use of scale models. Child Development, 71(2), 329–338. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00148

Google Scholar Crossref

Fennema, E. H. (1972). The relative effectiveness of a symbolic and a concrete model in learning a selected mathematical principle. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, (4), 233. https://doi.org/10.2307/748490

Google Scholar Crossref

Fuson, K. C. (2009). Avoiding misinterpretations of Piaget and Vygotsky: Mathematical teaching without learning, learning without teaching, or helpful learning-path teaching? Cognitive Development, 24(4), 343–361.

Google Scholar Crossref

Fyfe, E. R., DeCaro, M. S., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2014). An alternative time for telling: When conceptual instruction prior to problem solving improves mathematical knowledge. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(3), 502–519. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12035

Google Scholar Crossref

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Prentice, K., Burch, M., Hamlett, C. L., Owen, R., … Jancek, D. (2003). Explicitly teaching for transfer: Effects on third-grade students’ mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(2), 293–305. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.2.293

Google Scholar Crossref

Fyfe, E.R., Rittle-Johnson, B., & DeCaro, M.S. (2012). The effects of feedback during exploratory mathematics problem solving: Prior knowledge matters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(4), 1094-1108.

Google Scholar Crossref

Fyfe, E. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2016). The benefits of computer-generated feedback for mathematics problem solving. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 147, 140–151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.009

Google Scholar Crossref

Harcourt Assessment Inc. (2002). Stanford achievement test, tenth edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Inc.

Google Scholar Crossref

Horan, E. M. (2017). Guidance and structure in mathematics instruction: How much guidance do students need? An intervention study on kindergarten mathematics with manipulatives (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from https://galileo-usg-uga-primo.hosted.exlibrisgroup.com:443/UGA:UGA:01GALI_USG_ALMA71186079720002931

Google Scholar Crossref

Hunt, J. H. (2014). Effects of a Supplemental Intervention Focused in Equivalency Concepts for Students with Varying Abilities. Remedial and Special Education, 35(3), 135–144.

Google Scholar Crossref

Jitendra, A.K., Rodriguez, M., Kanive, R., Huang, J.-P., Church, C., Corroy, K.A., & Zaslofsky, A. (2013). Impact of small-group tutoring interventions on the mathematical problem solving and achievement of third-grade students with mathematics difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly, 36(1), 21-35. doi:10.1177/0731948712457561

Google Scholar Crossref

Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19(4), 509-539. doi: 10.1007/s10648-007-9054-3

Google Scholar Crossref

Kirschner, P.A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R.E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75-86.

Google Scholar Crossref

Kroesbergen, E. H., & van Luit, J. E. H. (2002). Teaching multiplication to low math performers: Guided versus structured instruction. Instructional Science, 30(5), 361–378. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1019880913714

Google Scholar Crossref

Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. H. (2005). Constructivist mathematics education for students with mild mental retardation. European Journal of Special Needs Education, 20(1), 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1080/0885625042000319115

Google Scholar Crossref

Laski, E. V., Jor’dan, J. R., Daoust, C., & Murray, A. K. (2015). What makes mathematics manipulatives effective? Lessons From cognitive science and montessori education. SAGE Open, 5(2), 1-8. doi: 10.1177/2158244015589588

Google Scholar Crossref

Martinez, R.S., Missall, K.N., Graney, S.B., Aricak, O.T., & Clarke, B. (2009). Technical adequacy of early numeracy curriculum-based measurement in kindergarten. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 34(2), 116-125. doi: 10.1177/1534508408326204

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (2004). Should there be a three-strikes rule against pure discovery learning? American Psychologist, 59(1), 14-19. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.14

Google Scholar Crossref

National Governors Association Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers. (2010). Common Core State Standards for Mathematics. Washington D.C.: National Governors Association Center for Best Practices, Council of Chief State School Officers. Retrieved from http://www.corestandards.org/the-standards

Google Scholar Crossref

National Research Council. (2009). Mathematics Learning in Early Childhood: Paths toward Excellence and Equity (No. 978-0-3091-2806–3). Washington, D.C.

Google Scholar Crossref

Piaget, J. (1977). Epistemology and psychology of functions. Boston, MA: D. Reidel Publishing Co.

Google Scholar Crossref

Ramani, G. B., Siegler, R. S., & Hitti, A. (2012). Taking it to the classroom: Number board games as a small group learning activity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 661–672. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028995

Google Scholar Crossref

Rittle-Johnson, B. (2006). Promoting transfer: Effects of self-explanation and direct instruction. Child Development, 77(1), 1-15.

Google Scholar Crossref

Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Salvia, J., & Ysseldyke, J.E. (2001). Assessment (8th ed.). Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

Google Scholar Crossref

Schwartz, J.E. (1992). "Silent teacher" and mathematics as reasoning. Arithmetic Teacher, 40(2), 122-124.

Google Scholar Crossref

Sowell, E. J. (1989). Effects of manipulative materials in mathematics instruction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20(5), 498–505. doi:10.2307/749423

Google Scholar Crossref

Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York, NY : Springer.

Google Scholar Crossref

Terwel, J., van Oers, B., van Dijk, I., & van den Eeden, P. (2009). Are representations to be provided or generated in primary mathematics education? Effects on transfer. Educational Research and Evaluation, 15(1), 25–44.

Google Scholar Crossref

Tournaki, N. (2003). The differential effects of teaching addition through strategy instruction versus drill and practice to students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(5), 449-458.

Google Scholar Crossref

Uttal, D. H., O’Doherty, K., Newland, R., Hand, L. L., & DeLoache, J. (2009). Dual representation and the linking of concrete and symbolic representations. Child Development Perspectives, 3(3), 156–159. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00097.x

Google Scholar Crossref

Vygotsky, L.S. (1978). Mind in society : The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Woodcock, R., & Johnson, M. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised. DLM Teaching Resources.

Google Scholar Crossref

Downloads

Published

2018-10-24

Almetric

Dimensions

How to Cite

Horan, E. M., & Carr, M. M. (2018). How Much Guidance Do Students Need? An Intervention Study on Kindergarten Mathematics with Manipulatives. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 7(3), 286–316. https://doi.org/10.17583/ijep.2018.3672

Issue

Section

Articles