Examining the Effects of Gender and Presentation Mode on Learning from a Multimedia Presentation

Authors

  • Fanni Liu Coward Texas Tech University
  • Steven M. Crooks Texas Tech University
  • Raymond Flores
  • Dan Dao

https://doi.org/10.4471/generos.2012.03

Keywords:


Downloads

Abstract

 

Visual presentation modes in multimedia learning include pictures, video, and animations. Research also reveals cognitive differences between males and females (Halpern, 2004). Which one of the presentation modes is more effective? Can one of these presentation modes be more effective for a specific gender? This study aimed to investigate the role of gender and presentation mode in multimedia learning. Participants were 72 university students randomly assigned to one of the two different versions of a computer-based multimedia program (narration with animation vs. narration with static images). A 2 × 2 factorial design is created by crossing gender and presentation mode (animation vs. static image). Dependent measures consisted of a transfer and a comprehension test. The results showed a significant modality by gender interaction on the comprehension test. Females performed better studying animations, whereas males performed better studying static pictures. The results are interpreted in light of multimedia learning principles and studies in the area of gender differences in learning. The important contribution of this study is the suggestion that individual differences such as gender should be considered in multimedia learning.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Author Biographies

Fanni Liu Coward, Texas Tech University

Assistant Professor

Department of Curriculum and Instruction

Steven M. Crooks, Texas Tech University

Associate Professor

Educational Psychology Department

References

Ayers, P. L., & Sweller, J. (2005). The split-attention principle in multimedia learning. In R. E Mayer (Ed.), Cambridge Handbook of Multimedia Learning (pp. 135-146). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon.

Google Scholar Crossref

Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556-559.

Google Scholar Crossref

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: a meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20, 327-344

Google Scholar Crossref

Berk, L. E. (2005) Infants, children, and adolescents (5th ed.) Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar Crossref

Berk, R. A. (2009). Multimedia teaching with video clips: TV, movies, YouTube, and mtvU in the college classroom. International Journal of Technology in Teaching and Learning, 5(1), 1–21.

Google Scholar Crossref

Bosco, A., Longoni, A., & Vecchi, T. (2004). Gender effect in spatial orientation: Cognitive profiles and mental strategies. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 519-532.

Google Scholar Crossref

Collins, D. W., & Kimura, D. (1997). A large sex difference on a two-dimensional rotation task. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111(4), 845-849.

Google Scholar Crossref

Clark, J.M., & Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education. Educational Psychology Review, 3, 149-210.

Google Scholar Crossref

Dong, Y., & Li, R. (2011). The reflection for multimedia teaching. Asian Social Science, 7(2), 165-167.

Google Scholar Crossref

Draper, S., & Anderson, A. (1991). The significant of dialogue in learning and observing learning. Computers & Education, 17, 93-107.

Google Scholar Crossref

Flores, R., Coward, F.L., & Crooks. S. (2011). Examining the influence of gender on

Google Scholar Crossref

the modality effect. Journal of Educational Technology System, 39(1), 87-103.

Google Scholar Crossref

Grimley, M. (2007). Learning from multimedia materials: The relative impact of individual differences. Educational Psychology, 27, 465-485.

Google Scholar Crossref

Ginns, P. (2005). Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction, 15, 113-331.

Google Scholar Crossref

Halpern, D.F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlnaum.

Google Scholar Crossref

Halpern, D.F. (2004). A cognitive-process taxonomy for sex differences in cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 135-139.

Google Scholar Crossref

Harper, S. F. & Mayer, R. E. (1997). The role of interest in learning from scientific text and illustrations: One the distinction between emotional interest and cognitive interest. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 92-102.

Google Scholar Crossref

Harper, S. F. & Mayer, R. E. (1997). How seductive details do their damage: A theory of cognitive interest in science learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 414-434.

Google Scholar Crossref

Herlitz, A, Nelsson, L.G. , & Backman, L. (1997). Gender differences in episodic memory. Memory and Cognition, 25, 801-811.

Google Scholar Crossref

Hildyard, A. and Olson, D. R. (1982). On the comprehension of oral vs written discourse. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and Literacy (pp.19-24). Norwood, N.J: Ablex

Google Scholar Crossref

Hoeffler, T., & Leutner, D. (2007). Instructional animation versus static pictures: A meta-analysis. Learning and Instruction, 17, 722 -738.

Google Scholar Crossref

Jonassen, D.H., & Grabowski, B. L. (1993). Handbook of individual differences, learning, and instructions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar Crossref

Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1998). Levels of expertise and instructional design. Human Factors, 40 (1), 1-17.

Google Scholar Crossref

Kim, S. S., Yoon, M. M., Whang, S. M., Tversky, B. B., & Morrison, J. B. (2007). The effect of animation on comprehension and interest. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 23(3), 260-270. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2006.00219.x

Google Scholar Crossref

Kimura, D. (1999) Sex and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Lai, Y., Tsai, H., & Yu, P. (2009). A multimedia English learning system using HMMs to Improve phonemic awareness for English learning. Journal Of Educational Technology & Society, 12(3), 266-281.

Google Scholar Crossref

Lanza, A., & Roselli, T. (1991). Effects of hypertextual approach versus structured approach on students’ achievement. Journal of Computer-Based Instruction,18(2), 48-50.

Google Scholar Crossref

Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessments(3rd ed.).New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Lin, H. (2011). Facilitating Learning from Animated Instruction: Effectiveness of Questions and Feedback as Attention-directing Strategies. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 14(2), 31-42.

Google Scholar Crossref

Lin, H. & Dwyer, F. M. (2010). The effect of static and animated visualization: a perspective of instructional effectiveness and efficiency. Educational Technology Research & Development, 58(2), 155-174.

Google Scholar Crossref

Maccoby, E. & Jacklin, C. (1974). The Psychology of Sex differences. Standford, Calif: Standford University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mann, B. L. (1997). Evaluation of presentation modalities in a hypermedia system. Computers Education, 28(2), 133-143.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (1978). Advance organizers that compensate for the organization of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 880-886.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (1979). Can advance organizers influence meaningful learning? Review of Educational Research, 49, 371-383.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (1980). Elaboration techniques that increase the meaningfulness of technical text: An experimental test of learning strategies hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 770-784.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (1981). The psychology of how novices learn computer programming. . Computing Surveys, 13, 121-141.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (1983). Can you read that? Qualitative effects of repetition and advanced organizers on learning from science prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 40-49.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (1997). Multimedia learning: Are we asking the right questions? Educational Psychologist, 32, 1-19.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (1999). Multimedia aids to problem solving transfer. International Journal of Educational Research, 31, 611-624.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia Learning. UK: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia Learning (2nd ed.). UK: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. (2003). The promise of multimedia learning: Using the same instructional design methods across different media. Learning & Instruction, 13, 125-139.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93 (1), 187-198.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E., & Massa, L.J. (2003). Three facets of visual and verbal learner: Cognitive ability, cognitive style and learning preferences. Journal of Educational Psychology,95, 833-846.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E. & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38 (1), 43-52.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mayer, R.E., & Sim, V.K. (1994). For whom is a picture worth a thousand words? Extension of a dual coding theory of multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 389-401.

Google Scholar Crossref

McGee, M.G. (1979). Human spatial abilities: Psychomentrix studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences. Psychological Bullentin, 86, 899-918.

Google Scholar Crossref

Merritt, P., Hirshamn, E., Wharton, W., Stangl, B., Devlin, J., & Lenz, A. (2007). Evidence for gender differences in visual selective attention. Personality and Individual Difference, 43, 597-609.

Google Scholar Crossref

Moreno, R., & Mayer, R.E. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 358-368.

Google Scholar Crossref

Mullis, I.V. S., Martin, M.O., Gonzalez, E., & Kennedy, A.M. (2003). PIRLS 2001 International Report: IEA’s study of reading literacy achievement in primary schools. Available online at http://timss.bc.edu/pirls2001i/PIRL2001_Pubs_IR.html

Google Scholar Crossref

Neo, M. & Neo,T. (2009). Engaging students in multimedia-mediated constructivist learning - Students' perceptions. Journal Of Educational Technology & Society, 12(2), 254-266.

Google Scholar Crossref

Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Google Scholar Crossref

Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representation: A dual-coding approach. New York: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar Crossref

Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255-287.

Google Scholar Crossref

Parette Jr., H. P., Hourcade, J., & Blum, C. (2011). Using Animation in Microsoft PowerPoint to Enhance Engagement and Learning in Young Learners With Developmental Delay. Teaching Exceptional Children, 43(4), 58-67.

Google Scholar Crossref

Penny, C. G. (1989). Modality effects and the structure of short-term verbal memory. Memory and Cognition, 17, 398-422.

Google Scholar Crossref

Quaiser-Pohl, C., & Lehmann, W. (2002). Girls’ spatial abilities: Charting the contributions of experiences and attitudes in different academic groups. British Journal of Educational Psychology,72, 245-260.

Google Scholar Crossref

Richardson, J.T.E. (1994). Gender differences in mental rotation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78,435-448.

Google Scholar Crossref

Riding, R.J., & Grimley, M. (1999). Cognitive style, gender, and learning from multimedia materials in 11-year-old children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 30, 43-56.

Google Scholar Crossref

Robinson, D. H., & Kiewra, K. A. (1995). Visual argument: Graphic organizers are superior to outlines in improving learning from text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 455-467.

Google Scholar Crossref

Robinson, D. H., & Molina (2002). The relative involvement of visual and auditory working memory when studying adjunct displays. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 118-131.

Google Scholar Crossref

Ruggiero, G., Sergi, I., & Iachini, T. (2008). Gender differences in remembering and inferring spatial distances. Memory, 16, 821-835.

Google Scholar Crossref

Rummer, R., Schweppe, J., Furstenberg, A., Seufert, T., & Brunken, R. (2010). Working memory interference during processing texts and pictures: Implications for the explanation of the modality effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 164-176.

Google Scholar Crossref

Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001) Imagery and text: A dual coding theory of reading and writing. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Google Scholar Crossref

Schmidt-Weigand, F., Kohnert, A., & Glowalla, U. (2010a). A closer look at split visual attention in system-and self-paced instruction in multimedia learning. Learning and Instruction, 20, 100-110.

Google Scholar Crossref

Schüeler, A., Scheiter, K., Gerjets, P., & Rummer, R. (2008a). Does a lack of contiguity with visual text cause the modality effect in multimedia learning? In B. C. Love, K. McRae, & V. M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2353-2358). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

Google Scholar Crossref

Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12, 185-233.

Google Scholar Crossref

Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and

Google Scholar Crossref

instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251-296.

Google Scholar Crossref

Tabbers, H., Martens, R., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (2004). Multimedia instructions and cognitive load theory: Effects of modality and cueing. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 71-81.

Google Scholar Crossref

Tan, U., Okuyan, M, Bayraktar, T, & Akgun, A. (2002). Sex difference in perceptual-verbal ability in relation to body size. International Journal of Neroscience, 112, 953-957.

Google Scholar Crossref

Taylor, M. M., Pountney, D. D., & Malabar, I. I. (2007). Animation as an aid for the teaching of mathematical concepts. Journal of Further & Higher Education, 31(3), 249-261.

Google Scholar Crossref

Vogel-Walcutt, J.J. Gebrim, J. B., & Nicholson, D. (2010). Animated versus static images of team processes to affect knowledge acquisition and learning efficiency. Journal of Online Learning and Teaching, 6(1), 1-11.

Google Scholar Crossref

Wouters, P., Paas, F., & van Merrienboer, J. J. G. (2009). Observational learning from animated models: Effects of modality and reflection on transfer. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 1-8.

Google Scholar Crossref

Yeu, H.K., & Goetz, E.T. (1994). Context effects on word recognition and reading comprehension of poor and good readers: A test of the interactive-compensatory hypothesis. Reading Research Quarterly, 29(2), 178-188.

Google Scholar Crossref

Downloads

Published

2012-02-24

Almetric

Dimensions

How to Cite

Coward, F. L., Crooks, S. M., Flores, R., & Dao, D. (2012). Examining the Effects of Gender and Presentation Mode on Learning from a Multimedia Presentation. Multidisciplinary Journal of Gender Studies, 1(1), 48–69. https://doi.org/10.4471/generos.2012.03

Issue

Section

Articles